Constraint-based Syntax 2: Week 3: Difference between revisions
(61 intermediate revisions by the same user not shown) | |||
Line 2: | Line 2: | ||
<font size="3"> | <font size="3"> | ||
<!-- | |||
== Maximal phrase types == | == Maximal phrase types == | ||
Line 51: | Line 52: | ||
* a state of affairs involving one entity, Lilly, who stands in the relationship that she snores (it is kind of awkward to say that something stands in a one-place relationship - it would be more intuitive in this case to say that Lilly has the property of snoring. But to keep the system general, we will also use the word relation when there is only a single object involved in the state of affairs.) | * a state of affairs involving one entity, Lilly, who stands in the relationship that she snores (it is kind of awkward to say that something stands in a one-place relationship - it would be more intuitive in this case to say that Lilly has the property of snoring. But to keep the system general, we will also use the word relation when there is only a single object involved in the state of affairs.) | ||
Note that a state of affairs is completely abstract: all by itself, nothing is said about whether there is such a state of affairs or not | Note that a state of affairs is completely abstract: all by itself, nothing is said about whether there is such a state of affairs or not and nothing is said about whether somebody believes, claims, denies, or dreams of such a state of affairs! This is precisely the crucial difference between non-clauses and clauses: non-clauses such as verbs and verb phrases have such abstract states of affairs as their content. In contrast, clauses express '''messages about a state of affairs'''. One type of message is '''proposition''', the claim that a state of affairs actually holds. Thus, the sentence ''Lilly chases Fido'' expresses the proposition that ''there is'' a state of affairs where she chases him and by uttering this sentence, a speaker actually commits herself to the truth of the proposition, i.e. to the claim that the world corresponds to the state of affairs described in the sentence she uttered. In our case, this means that by uttering the sentence ''Lilly chases Fido'', the speaker claims that it is true that she chases him. | ||
Each message type is grammatically associated with a distinct '''clause type''' (= a conventional combination of syntax and semantics) by the grammar of English. Since there are four message types, there also are four clause types. The table below illustrates the one-to-one relationship between message and clause types: | |||
{| class="wikitable" | {| class="wikitable" | ||
|- | |- | ||
! Clause type !! Full name !! | ! Clause type !! Full name !! Message type !! Examples | ||
|- | |- | ||
| ''decl-clause'' || declarative clause || '' | | ''decl-clause'' || declarative clause || ''proposition'' || Lilly danced. | ||
|- | |- | ||
| ''inter-clause'' || interrogative clause || ''question'' || Does Lilly dance?, Who danced? | | ''inter-clause'' || interrogative clause || ''question'' || Does Lilly dance?, Who danced? | ||
Line 74: | Line 70: | ||
|} | |} | ||
The first case has already been discussed. The remaining cases are analogous. An interrogative clause expresses the message '''question'''. By uttering such a clause, the speaker can ask whether the state of affairs which is part of the message holds or doesn't hold. An imperative clause expresses the '''outcome''' that the speaker wishes the addressee to bring about i.e. which state of affairs should come to hold as a result of the speaker uttering this type of clause to one or more hearers. Finally, an exclamative clause expresses a fact which the speaker considers surprising or unusual. | |||
In the following sections, we will present a number of constraints on non-clausal and clausal phrases that interact to define the four types of head-complement phrases and the four types of clauses introduced in this section. | |||
== Constraints on all Phrases == | |||
There are 3 simple principles that apply to all phrases, clausal and non-clausal alike. | |||
=== Empty COMPS Constraint (ECC) === | |||
[[File:ECC.jpg]] | |||
Informally: phrases have empty COMPS values. | |||
=== Head Feature Principle === | |||
[[File:HFP.jpg]] | |||
Informally: phrases and their head daughters have identical HEAD features. | |||
=== Background Inheritance Principle === | |||
[[File:bckgrnd-principle.jpg]] | |||
Informally: phrases and their head daughters have identical values for the feature BCKGRND. | |||
=== The 3 Phrase Principles Combined === | |||
[[File:phrase-constraint.jpg]] | |||
--> | |||
== The Phrase Hierarchy == | |||
[[File:phrase-hierarchy.jpg]] | |||
== Constraints on the Phrasal Types == | |||
[[File:constraint-phrase.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-hd-ph.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-fin-vp.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-nf-hc-ph.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-clause.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-core-cl.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-decl-cl.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-decl-hd-su-cl.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-decl-ns-cl.jpg]] | |||
[[File:constraint-cp-cl.jpg]] | |||
=== Illustration of Constraint Inheritance === | |||
Since the type type{fin-vp} has several phrasal supertypes, a feature structure of that type must satisfy the following constraint: | |||
<br> | |||
[[File:constraint-fin-vp-plus-supertypes.jpg]] | |||
== Exercises == | |||
1. Using the handout [[File:Phrasal Types.pdf|frame|'''Phrasal Types''']], compute by hand the overall constraints that the maximal subtypes of type ''phrase'' have to satisfy (leave out the type ''decl-ns-cl''). In other words: write into one feature structure all the constraints of each maximal subtype and its supertypes in the type hierarchy. | |||
2. By parsing the following expressions in the [http://141.2.159.95:7003/wt/ Schematic Grammar], you can test the constraints associated with the type ''phrase'' and its subtypes, in particular the 4 maximal types whose overall constraints you were supposed to compute by hand in Exercise 1: | |||
a. word subj_seeker<br> | |||
b. word spr_seeker<br> | |||
c. comp_seeker word<br> | |||
<br> | |||
3. By parsing the following expressions in our [http://141.2.159.95:7002/wt/ Regular Online Grammar], you will see which information authentic lexical entries contribute to a phrase: | |||
a. Real "subject seekers": | |||
* dances | |||
* likes Lilly | |||
b. Real "specifier seekers": | |||
* cat | |||
* picture of Lilly | |||
c. Real "complement seekers": | |||
* likes Lilly | |||
* puts the books on the shelf | |||
After you have tried the examples above, think up 3 more examples for each of the 3 types and try them out in the regular online grammar. | |||
Line 172: | Line 229: | ||
This is a typical modal verb. Like the other modals, it has only finite forms. Again you get two solutions, a polarized and a non-polarized one. An intriguing difference between ''will'' and the unstressed ''do'' discussed above, is that ''will'' can occur in auxiliary constructions (and then is [AUX ''plus'']), but it can also occur in constructions where main verbs can occur as well (and then, like the main verbs, will be [AUX ''minus'']. In order to be compatible with both values of the feature AUX, ''will'', it takes the neutral value [AUX ''boolean''] in its lexical entry. All verbs that can appear in auxiliary constructions except for the forms of the unstressed ''do'' behave like ''will''. | This is a typical modal verb. Like the other modals, it has only finite forms. Again you get two solutions, a polarized and a non-polarized one. An intriguing difference between ''will'' and the unstressed ''do'' discussed above, is that ''will'' can occur in auxiliary constructions (and then is [AUX ''plus'']), but it can also occur in constructions where main verbs can occur as well (and then, like the main verbs, will be [AUX ''minus'']. In order to be compatible with both values of the feature AUX, ''will'', it takes the neutral value [AUX ''boolean''] in its lexical entry. All verbs that can appear in auxiliary constructions except for the forms of the unstressed ''do'' behave like ''will''. | ||
== The Argument Realization Principle (ARP) === | |||
Recall that in GS the syntactic arguments of a word are in a sense represented twice: once on the ARG-ST (argument structure) list which, among others, is used for Binding Theory, and again on one of the valence lists SUBJ, SPR, and COMPS. Rather than being arbitrary, the relationship between the ARG-S and the valence lists is governed by the '''Argument Realization Principle''': | Recall that in GS the syntactic arguments of a word are in a sense represented twice: once on the ARG-ST (argument structure) list which, among others, is used for Binding Theory, and again on one of the valence lists SUBJ, SPR, and COMPS. Rather than being arbitrary, the relationship between the ARG-S and the valence lists is governed by the '''Argument Realization Principle''': | ||
Line 202: | Line 254: | ||
Navigation: | Navigation: | ||
<div align="center"> | <div align="center"> | ||
[[Constraint-based_Syntax_2| '''Main page''']] [[Constraint-based_Syntax_2:_Week_1| | [[Constraint-based_Syntax_2| '''Main page''']] [[Constraint-based_Syntax_2:_Week_1| '''Week 1''']] [[Constraint-based_Syntax_2:_Week_2| '''Week 2''']] [[Constraint-based_Syntax_2:_Week_3| '''Week 3''']] [[Constraint-based_Syntax_2:_Week_4| '''Week 4''']] [[Constraint-based_Syntax_2:_Week_5| '''Week 5''']] Week6 Week7 Week8 Week9 Week10 | ||
</div> | </div> | ||
Latest revision as of 10:02, 16 May 2017
The Phrase Hierarchy
Constraints on the Phrasal Types
Illustration of Constraint Inheritance
Since the type type{fin-vp} has several phrasal supertypes, a feature structure of that type must satisfy the following constraint:
Exercises
1. Using the handout File:Phrasal Types.pdf, compute by hand the overall constraints that the maximal subtypes of type phrase have to satisfy (leave out the type decl-ns-cl). In other words: write into one feature structure all the constraints of each maximal subtype and its supertypes in the type hierarchy.
2. By parsing the following expressions in the Schematic Grammar, you can test the constraints associated with the type phrase and its subtypes, in particular the 4 maximal types whose overall constraints you were supposed to compute by hand in Exercise 1:
a. word subj_seeker
b. word spr_seeker
c. comp_seeker word
3. By parsing the following expressions in our Regular Online Grammar, you will see which information authentic lexical entries contribute to a phrase:
a. Real "subject seekers":
- dances
- likes Lilly
b. Real "specifier seekers":
- cat
- picture of Lilly
c. Real "complement seekers":
- likes Lilly
- puts the books on the shelf
After you have tried the examples above, think up 3 more examples for each of the 3 types and try them out in the regular online grammar.
Navigation: